Academic Motivation Scale: adaptation and psychometric analyses for high school and college students (2024)

  • Journal List
  • Psychol Res Behav Manag
  • v.5; 2012
  • PMC3414248

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Academic Motivation Scale: adaptation and psychometric analyses for high school and college students (1)

Dove Medical PressThis ArticleSubscribeSubmit a ManuscriptSearchFollowDovepressPsychology Research and Behavior Management

Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2012; 5: 71–83.

Published online 2012 Jul 25. doi:10.2147/PRBM.S33188

PMCID: PMC3414248

PMID: 22888280

Juliana Beatriz Stover,1 Guadalupe de la Iglesia,1 Antonio Rial Boubeta,2 and Mercedes Fernández Liporace1

Author information Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

Abstract

The Academic Motivation Scale (AMS), supported in Self-Determination Theory, has been applied in recent decades as well in high school as in college education. Although several versions in Spanish are available, the underlying linguistic and cultural differences raise important issues when they are applied to Latin-American population. Consequently an adapted version of the AMS was developed, and its construct validity was analyzed in Argentine students. Results obtained on a sample that included 723 students from Buenos Aires (393 high school and 330 college students) verified adequate psychometric properties in this new version, solving some controversies regarded to its dimensionality.

Keywords: Academic Motivation, self-determination, confirmatory factor analysis, internal consistency

Introduction

One of the most studied topics in educational psychology is academic motivation. Its direct and indirect influence on learning processes has been proven in numerous investigations.14 On one hand, it has been linked to the effort and persistence invested in the depth of information processing and to the use of self-regulated cognitive and metacognitive strategies.5 On the other hand, its close association with self-efficacy perception, goal establishment, and generation of achievement expectations has been pointed out repeatedly.6,7 Previous investigations have remarked on how motivation modulates valorization of assigned tasks and marks the pertinence of using external rewards.8,9 As well as that, it has been suggested that low achievement, desertion, and difficulties in the transition between educational levels are often evidence of problems that, in order to design palliative interventions, may be targeted from a motivational perspective.10,11

As a consequence of its complexity and its central role in the educational milieu, the need for the development of instruments capable of providing researchers and professionals valid and reliable measures of this construct has been stressed.12 Most of the scales have been based on different conceptual proposals. The corpus that was used to give shape to a good amount of the research in this field is the self-determination theory, which has its origin in the work of Deci and Ryan.13 This is a macrotheory related to human development and the function of personality in different social contexts. It considers that motivation can be expressed through a continuum of increasing self-determination with three fundamental positions reflecting the degree of autonomy on which behaviors are based: amotivation and extrinsic and intrinsic motivation (Figure 1).

Open in a separate window

Figure 1

Self-determination continuum based on Deci and Ryan13 and Vallerand et al.14

Abbreviations: AMS, Academic Motivation Scale; IMse, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards stimulating experiences; IMa, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards achievement; IMk, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards knowledge; EMidr, Extrinsic Motivation identified regulation; EMintr, Extrinsic Motivation introjected regulation; EMer, Extrinsic Motivation external regulation; A, Amotivation.

Amotivation represents the nonregulated extreme of the continuum, and it is characterized by the individual’s perception of lack of control over events, incompetence and absence of purpose. In extrinsic motivation (EM), which is situated in the midpoint of the mentioned continuum, the goal being chased constitutes the main driving force of behavior, which is divided into four subtypes of progressive regulation: external, introjected, identified, and integrated. In the first of them, behaviors are enforced by others and are carried out to avoid punishment or to obtain rewards. In introjected regulation, behaviors are executed in order to improve one’s self-esteem or to avoid anxiety and guilt that may arise for not carrying them out. In identified regulation, the individual chooses the activities by extrinsic motives (eg, society values enrolling in superior studies). The last subtype (integrated regulation) would appear only in adulthood, when individual needs and values converge with those expected by the social context (eg, studying broadens one’s horizons). Finally, intrinsic motivation (IM) occupies the autonomous regulation pole where the pleasure of executing behaviors by own choice prevails: activities become a goal themselves.

Years later, Vallerand et al14 expanded this initial description introducing a new division of three subtypes of IM referred to in the academic milieu: orientation towards knowledge (the task is carried out for the pleasure of learning), orientation towards achievement (satisfaction emerges when products are generated or when one’s own limits are overcome), and orientation towards stimulating experiences (it involves activities developed to perceive comforting aesthetics, intellectual or sensorial sensations) (Figure 1).14

In regard of its assessment, the Academic Motivation Scale (AMS) is one of the most used instruments.14 Aimed at adolescents and adults in academic environments, its original French and English versions have 28 items distributed in seven subscales that respond to the dimensions referenced in self-determination theory. The subtype titled EM-integrated regulation is an exception which was excluded as it appears later in adult life. The seven-factor structure, postulated in the initial theoretical model from Vallerand et al, was verified by subsequent confirmatory analyses and showed adequate internal consistency.14,15 Research carried out in Spain, the United States of America, Greece, and Paraguay focused mostly on college students and found a better statistic fit for this seven-factor model in contrast with five-, three-, and one-factor solutions.1621 The five-factor structure corresponds with the original proposal from Deci and Ryan.13 The three-factor model was added to confirm if MI and ME dimensions could be grouped. Finally, unidimensionality was tested as the simplest alternative.

The use of the AMS has allowed researchers to distinguish relevant associations between motivation and academic variables. This way, for example, students who showed a greater self-determination level also achieved higher quality learning with a better academic record, showed more interest in class, a greater perceived competence and, as a general rule, better attendance at school.2225

The existing literature describes an instrument with remarkable theoretical and psychometric strength that permits a valid and reliable assessment of the construct. However, cultural differences inherent to diverse populations over which it was expected to be used, as well as contextual dissimilarities between educational levels, warn about the need to review the wording or interpretation of some of its items before using them. Núñez et al, for example, revealed problems in the use of Spanish in Latin America, verifying regional semantic connotations associated to some terms.20 These difficulties end up affecting the psychometric functioning in terms of its factor structure and internal consistency.

The present research was stimulated by the significant interest that the assessment of motivation has in the educational milieu and the need to have adapted and validated versions of the AMS available in different countries and cultural contexts. Therefore, we attempted to validate empirically two versions of the scale for its use with Argentine high school and college students. The main goal is to make two versions of the AMS available for researchers and professionals, which will guarantee the reliability and validity standards usually required for all measures. In a complementary manner, and following previous research, the psychometric analysis of the goodness of fit of one-, three-, five-, and seven-factor models will give even more empirical evidence about the dimensionality of the construct.

Methods

Participants

The sample was composed of 723 students of public and private institutions in Buenos Aires who were of middle socioeconomic status. Even though sampling was not randomized, the distribution by sex and age reflected the proportions referred by the Argentine Educational Ministry.26 The high school group was composed of 393 students (64.9% female; 35.1% male; aged between 13 and 19 years; mean [M] = 15.24; standard deviation [SD] = 1.60). The college group was composed of 330 individuals (68.1% female; 31.9% male; aged between 17 and 35 years; M = 23.45; SD = 4.47). In order to gain the maximum possible representation, students of different schools were included (psychology, medicine, veterinary, engineering, chemistry, and literature departments), also considering the official data of percentage of students in each career.

Instruments and procedure

A version of the AMS into Spanish for Argentine subjects was used for data recollection.14

Data were gathered by collective administration of the AMS scale in the classrooms of each institution. Students or the parents of underage participants signed informed consent statements. Students were not paid for their participation in the study. Anonymity and data confidential treatment were guaranteed at all times. An Institutional Ethics Committee of the University of Buenos Aires, Psychology Department, backed up the current research with a corresponding written report which was in concordance of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Data analysis

Expert judgment was analyzed by the calculation of Aiken indexes. Descriptive statistics (M, SDs, skewedness, and kurtosis indexes) were calculated in order to study item distribution at a univariate level. When assessing multivariate normality, Mardia’s coefficient was examined. A confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to study the factorial structure of the scale. Given the nonnormality of data, unweighted least squares (ULS) was the chosen method. In a complementary manner, a bootstrap procedure using 500 random samples was applied to attempt to guarantee stability in the results. Finally, Cronbach’s alphas as well as Elosúa and Zumbo’s ordinal version of the Cronbach’s alpha were estimated to inform factor consistency.27 To empirically calibrate the suitability of the use of each specific version, differentiated analyses for each group or academic level were completed. Analyses were carried out by PASW Statistic (v 18.00; SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL) and AMOS (v 18.0; SPSS Inc).

Results

Content analysis

A back-translation of the English version of the AMS was made to achieve an adequate adaptation of the original scale to the context and linguistic peculiarities of the Spanish spoken in Argentina. This task considered the contents of both French and Spanish versions. Given that the original English and French versions were aimed at college students, items were adapted to high school students (eg, I can’t understand what am I doing in college became I can’t understand what am I doing in school). Versions for each educational level were obtained.

Following the work of Núñez et al, emphasis was placed on studying the content of each element at different stages.20 A pilot study was conducted with 25 college and 15 high school students. Results showed that some terms, such as pleasure and satisfaction, triggered interpretations unrelated to the academic environment. As a consequence of this finding, the terms were replaced by terms with neutral connotations (eg, Because I enjoy learning new things was used instead of For the pleasure and the satisfaction I experience while I learn new things). The seven-point Likert scale was reduced to a four-point one (1, strongly disagree; 2, disagree; 3, agree; and 4, strongly agree) that was more intuitive for our students and provided semantic anchorage points that considerably facilitated their response.

Then, five experts in psychometrics and educational psychology gave their individual judgments, which promoted additional modifications of some items and subscales. It was pointed out that EM-introjected regulation only contemplated items that referred to the ego. Their advice resulted in the substitution of two items on guilty feelings (eg, going to college so as not to disappoint their family), which favored a more complete operationalization of the underlying dimension.

In the same way, it was suggested that IM-stimulating experiences included only activities of learning through reading. Considering this observation, one of the original items was replaced by another one referring to practical tasks (eg, conducting an activity related to a future profession). This way, the instrument could account for the diversity of activities according to the student’s planned career after graduation. Finally, 28 items had Aiken coefficients equal or higher than 0.80, which ensured content validity of both specific versions for each educational level.

Construct validity

In addition to their means and standard deviations, standardized values of skewedness and kurtosis of each item were analyzed. Tables 1 and ​and22 show that most items had values over ±1.96. On a multivariate level, kurtosis values above 5.99 were found in both groups (critical ratio high-school [CRHS] = 32.914; critical ratio college [CRC] = 89.078). Therefore, the null hypothesis of normal distribution was rejected in both uni0 and multivariate analyses. As a consequence, the method of choice for the factorial study was ULS since, according to experts, this method does not have any assumptions about the distribution, is more robust in small samples, and is recommended for ordinal variables.28,29

Table 1

Item analysis and descriptive statistics of the high school sample

M (SD)SkewednessKurtosis
ValueETz (G1)ValueETz (G1)
IMse
Item 12.29 (0.922)−1.6100.123−13.0892.2920.2469.317
Item 82.15 (0.930)0.4580.1233.943−0.6210.246−2.524
Item 152.19 (0.904)0.4120.1233.34−0.5670.246−2.304
Item 222.30 (1.063)0.3070.1232.4951.1280.2464.585
IMa
Item 22.35 (0.886)0.2170.1231.764−0.6530.246−1.028
Item 92.52 (0.934)0.0840.1230.682−0.8730.246−3.548
Item 162.49 (0.980)0.0480.1230.390−0.9980.246−4.056
Item 232.61 (0.920)0.0320.1230.260−0.8790.246−3.573
IMk
Item 32.36 (0.809)0.3630.1232.951−0.2930.246−1.190
Item 102.38 (0.876)0.2130.1231.731−0.6200.246−2.520
Item 172.67 (0.944)−0.1500.123−1.219−0.8910.246−3.621
Item 242.93 (0.868)−0.1960.123−1.593−0.9990.246−4.060
EMidr
Item 42.29 (0.922)−1.1670.123−9.4870.8240.2463.349
Item 113.21 (0.922)−0.9220.123−7.495−0.9180.246−3.731
Item 183.19 (0.869)−0.7960.123−6.471−0.2250.246−0.0914
Item 253.31 (0.818)−0.9990.123−8.1210.2480.2461.008
EMintr
Item 52.58 (1.059)−0.0880.123−0.715−1.2100.246−4.918
Item 122.73 (0.979)−0.1930.123−1.569−1.0020.246−4.073
Item 192.47 (0.987)−1.1720.123−9.528−1.1720.246−4.764
Item 262.92 (1.077)−0.5200.123−4.227−1.0580.246−4.300
EMer
Item 63.54 (0.713)−1.6100.123−13.0892.2920.2469.317
Item 133.54 (0.762)−1.6370.123−13.3081.9930.2468.101
Item 203.34 (0.827)−0.9720.123−7.902−0.0650.246−0.264
Item 271.90 (1.112)0.9010.1237.325−0.6410.246−2.605
A
Item 71.28 (0.629)2.5620.12320.9296.6200.24626.910
Item 141.25 (0.614)2.7920.12322.6997.8860.24632.056
Item 211.27 (0.622)2.5420.12320.6662.5320.24610.292
Item 281.21 (0.564)2.9980.12324.3739.0520.24636.796

Open in a separate window

Abbreviations: IMse, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards stimulating experiences; IMa, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards achievement; IMk, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards knowledge; EMidr, Extrinsic Motivation identified regulation; EMintr, Extrinsic Motivation introjected regulation; EMer, Extrinsic Motivation external regulation; A, Amotivation. M, mean; SD, standard deviation.

Table 2

Item analysis and descriptive statistics of the college sample

M (SD)SkewednessKurtosis coefficient
ValueETz (G1)ValueETz (G1)
IMse
Item 12.5 (0.910)0.2070.1341.544−0.7910.268−2.951
Item 82.33 (0.924)−0.3720.134−2.291−0.8330.268−3.108
Item 153.01 (0.831)−0.3370.134−2.514−0.7570.268−2.824
Item 223.12 (0.816)−0.5640.134−4.208−0.4240.268−1.582
IMa
Item 23.06 (0.823)−0.4420.134−3.298−0.6070.268−2.264
Item 92.94 (0.892)−0.4030.134−3.007−0.7010.268−2.615
Item 162.60 (0.931)0.0350.1340.261−0.9070.268−3.384
Item 232.90 (0.862)−0.3150.134−2.350−0.6670.268−1.062
IMk
Item 33.33 (0.746)−0.7170.134−5.350−0.5580.268−2.082
Item 103.18 (0.810)−0.6260.134−4.671−0.4440.268−1.656
Item 173.25 (0.778)−0.7310.134−5.455−0.1520.268−0.567
Item 243.37 (0.691)−0.7490.134−5.589−0.1630.268−0.608
EMidr
Item 43.48 (0.780)−1.3480.134−8.4680.8770.2683.272
Item 113.46 (0.744)−1.2370.134−9.2310.8650.2683.227
Item 182.57 (1.047)−0.0420.134−0.313−1.1890.268−4.436
Item 253.28 (0.773)−0.8450.134−6.3050.1570.2680.585
EMintr
Item 52.24 (0.981)0.3140.1342.343−0.9110.268−3.399
Item 121.73 (0.876)0.9840.1347.3430.0680.2680.253
Item 192.10 (1.131)0.5450.1344.067−1.1350.268−4.235
Item 262.90 (0.862)1.1580.1348.6410.3520.2681.313
EMer
Item 62.86 (0.934)−0.2430.134−1.813−0.9680.268−3.611
Item 132.92 (0.924)−0.4030.134−3.007−0.8390.268−3.130
Item 203.34 (0.827)0.0150.1340.111−0.9930.268−3.705
Item 271.07 (0.350)5.7940.13443.23837.7320.268140.792
A
Item 71.07 (0.337)5.9990.13444.76841.2880.268154.059
Item 141.17 (0.493)3.5700.13426.64114.3420.26853.518
Item 211.03 (0.262)9.6140.13471.746101.1550.268377.444
Item 281.04 (231)8.4460.13463.03288.1190.268328.802

Open in a separate window

Abbreviations: IMse, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards stimulating experiences; IMa, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards achievement; IMk, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards knowledge; EMidr, Extrinsic Motivation identified regulation; EMintr, Extrinsic Motivation introjected regulation; EMer, Extrinsic Motivation external regulation; A, Amotivation. M, mean; SD, standard deviation.

Other researchers analyzed one-, three-, five-, and seven-factor models for their goodness-of-fit.1618,21 Following Byrne and Kline, on the assessment of global fit, different indexes were considered simultaneously (Table 3).30,31

Table 3

Goodness-of-fit indexes obtained by ULS

GFIAGFIRMRNFIRFI
7 factors
HS0.9620.9530.0510.9440.935
C0.9530.9420.0450.9200.908
5 factors
HS0.9560.9470.0550.9340.927
C0.9300.9170.0550.8810.868
3 factors
HS0.9430.9430.0630.9160.908
C0.9190.9050.0590.8620.850
1 factor
HS0.9250.9130.0720.8880.879
C0.8420.8170.0830.7320.710

Open in a separate window

Abbreviations: AGFI, Adjusted Goodness-of-Fit Index; C, college students; GFI, Goodness-of-Fit Index; HS, high school students; NFI, Normal Fit Index; RFI, Relative Fix Index; RMR, root mean square residual; ULS, unweighted least squares.

The Goodness-of-Fit Index (GFI) estimates the error committed when reproducing the variance and covariance matrix. The Adjusted Goodness-of-Fit Index (AGFI) differs in that it adjusts considering the degrees of freedom of the specified model. The Normal Fit Index (NFI) and Relative Fit Index (RFI) compare the hypothesized model with the null model. They provide a complete measure of data variation. All of these indexes must show values over 0.90 to confirm the model. Root mean square residual (RMR) represents an average of the residual values resulting from the fitting of the variance– covariance matrix with the variance–covariance matrix of the hypothesized model. Values under 0.05 are acceptable.

GFI and AGFI showed values over 0.90 for models of seven-, five-, and three-factors in both groups, being always higher in the seven-factor model. RFI and NFI showed values higher than 0.90 in both groups just for the seven-factor model. Finally, RMR reached acceptable values for the seven- and five-factor models, with better values for the seven-factor model.

Model respecification

After examining the parameters of the seven-factor model, item 27 (EM-external regulation subscale) was left out because its factorial weight was nonsignificant in both groups and this affected internal consistency. Also, some correlations between latent variables were nonsignificant, especially in the college student group. The respecification of the model by eliminating item 27 resulted in a considerable increase of goodness of fit. In the high school student group, the GFI was 0.973, AGFI was 0.967, NFI was 0.961, RFI was 0.955, and RMR was 0.051. In the college student group, GFI was 0.955, AGFI was 0.944, NFI was 0.923, RFI was 0.911, and RMR was 0.046. A complementary analysis using maximum likelihood estimation was carried out to obtain χ2 values. This allowed a comparison of model 1 (28 items) with model 2 (27 items). Despite the fact that the χ2 estimation is still significant, an important decrease in the value of the estimation occurs in the high school student group (Model 1: χ2 = 769.1; df = 329; Model 2: χ2 = 669; gl = 303; Δχ2 = 100.1; P < 0.001) and the college student group (Model 1: χ2 = 791.1; gl = 329; Model 2: χ2 = 680; gl = 303; Δχ2 = 110.3; P < 0.001). Model 2 (27-item version) was chosen as the best alternative.

Tables 4 and ​and55 contain the λ and ϕ parameters obtained by bootstrap, with mean values and confidence intervals of 90%. All parameters were significant, with factorial weights mostly between 0.60 and 0.70. In the college student group, Item 21 (Amotivation scale) had a lower factorial weight, but it was not discarded in order to obtain similar scales for both groups by contemplating specific item contents, but also ensuring specificity by educational level. The calculated corrected hom*ogeneity indexes for each dimension had acceptable values (between 0.30 and 0.71) (Tables 4 and ​and55).

Table 4

Standardized factorial weights obtained by bootstrap

High school studentsCHICollege studentsCHI
MInferiorSuperiorPMInferiorSuperiorP
IMse
AMS 10.6090.5460.6840.0030.5690.6520.5750.7150.0070.575
AMS 80.7220.6600.7780.0040.6540.6250.5360.7090.0030.519
AMS 150.7090.6570.7580.0040.5860.6450.5640.7180.0050.519
AMS 220.7450.6980.7930.0040.6050.5730.4800.6640.0050.398
IMa
AMS 20.7280.6770.7740.0040.6750.6440.5830.7140.0030.632
AMS 90.7350.6940.7900.0020.6990.6540.5830.7220.0040.604
AMS 160.7140.6580.7710.0030.5980.7160.6310.7810.0050.600
AMS 230.7270.6830.7830.0020.5950.7240.6480.7840.0050.516
IMk
AMS 30.6500.5810.7110.0050.5840.6000.5820.7240.0050.571
AMS 100.7480.6970.7940.0040.6940.6840.6010.7460.0070.609
AMS 170.7510.7060.7910.0040.6550.7270.6390.7950.0070.648
AMS 240.7110.6610.7660.0020.5720.7610.6860.8240.0050.610
EMidr
AMS 40.5070.4300.6400.0020.3670.5430.4470.6370.0030.409
AMS 110.4670.3830.5540.0040.3370.5240.4130.6180.0040.469
AMS 180.5130.4180.5960.0050.3740.4800.3820.5760.0040.285
AMS 250.6340.5640.7000.0040.4740.6270.5370.7060.0040.493
EMintr
AMS 50.6670.5890.7510.0020.3650.5710.4550.6550.0070.387
AMS 120.6930.6190.7810.0030.4890.5140.4210.6150.0030.386
AMS 190.4580.3340.5520.0060.4300.5610.4470.6430.0090.394
AMS 260.3520.2340.4420.0070.4220.4650.3290.5430.0090.398
EMer
AMS 60.3740.2440.5010.0040.4200.5730.4800.6490.0070.528
AMS 130.8420.7710.9260.0020.5560.8350.7880.8990.0020.626
AMS 200.7350.6620.8190.0020.5930.7720.6940.8290.0060.656
A
AMS 70.7620.6590.8680.0030.4580.8910.7291.1420.0010.715
AMS 140.5390.4090.6740.0030.4980.6430.3140.8140.0110.580
AMS 210.7090.5630.8320.0040.7200.4630.0690.8810.1420.523
AMS 280.6900.5820.7930.0040.6790.7770.5841.0410.0010.703

Open in a separate window

Abbreviations: AMS, Academic Motivation Scale item; CHI, corrected hom*ogeneity indexes; IMse, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards stimulating experiences; IMa, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards achievement; IMk, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards knowledge; EMidr, Extrinsic Motivation identified regulation; EMintr, Extrinsic Motivation introjected regulation; EMer, Extrinsic Motivation external regulation; A, motivation.

Table 5

Correlations between factors obtained by bootstrap

High school studentsCollege students
ValueInferiorSuperiorPValueInferiorSuperiorP
IMse
IMa0.8360.7810.8840.0050.7410.6560.8060.006
IMa0.8980.9420.9410.0070.8920.8320.9470.005
EMidr0.5960.4850.6670.0060.3850.2470.5100.006
EMintr0.5040.4140.5950.0030.2490.1130.3900.006
EMer0.1450.0400.2640.0320.093−0.0240.2160.190
A−0.403−0.500−0.2800.006−0.140−0.2720.0450.174
IMa
IMa0.8130.7450.8570.0090.7220.6420.7860.005
EMidr0.7360.6610.8060.0040.5290.4050.6140.010
EMintr0.7020.6200.7820.0040.5840.4570.6830.004
EMer0.3480.2540.4410.0030.3970.2640.4950.006
A−0.416−0.519−0.2980.004−0.185−0.294−0.0200.068
IMk
EMidr0.6800.5990.7630.0040.3780.2420.5090.006
EMintr0.5030.4040.5860.0050.059−0.0920.1940.525
EMer0.1900.0720.3030.0070.095−0.0370.2090.248
A−0.442−0.549−0.3060.007−0.253−0.405−0.0330.044
EMidr
EMintr0.6540.5300.7680.0050.6320.4890.7600.004
EMer0.7690.6730.8600.0050.7920.6900.8810.005
A−0.472−0.596−0.3650.003−0.336−0.473−0.1380.016
EMintr
EMer0.5970.4860.6800.0070.6840.5950.7800.003
A−0.235−0.362−0.1260.0030.027−0.0630.1760.582
EMer
A−0.116−0.2500.0000.098−0.122−0.2280.0630.275

Open in a separate window

Abbreviations: IMse, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards stimulating experiences; IMa, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards achievement; IMk, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards knowledge; EMidr, Extrinsic Motivation identified regulation; EMintr, Extrinsic Motivation introjected regulation; EMer, Extrinsic Motivation external regulation; A, Amotivation.

As for the correlations between factors, in most cases the values estimated resulted significant (especially in the high school student group). The highest indexes were found between the dimensions located closer in the self-determination continuum, as this theory established ( especially in the three IM dimensions). The lowest were found between the most remote dimensions (IM-stimulating experiences and amotivation). Higher correlations were found in the scales of IM in comparison to those of EM. Between EM-identified regulation (EMidr) and EM-external regulation (EMer) strongest correlations were verified, compared to EM-introjected regulation (EMintr), which is located in the theoretical continuum between these two (Figures 2 and ​and33).

Open in a separate window

Figure 2

High school sample: estimated parameters.

Open in a separate window

Figure 3

College sample: estimated parameters.

Consistency of the factors

Cronbach’s alpha values ranged between 0.60 and 0.81 (Table 6). Though ordinal alphas showed indexes practically identical (between 0.59 and 0.82), they were in general a bit higher in all dimensions.

Table 6

Internal consistency for dimensions

GroupIMseIMaIMkEMidrEMintrEMerA
Cronbach’s alphaHS0.7920.8190.8080.6050.6450.7020.778
C0.7140.7810.7970.6200.6080.7690.775
Ordinal alphaHS0.790.820.800.610.620.670.77
C0.710.770.790.590.680.770.80

Open in a separate window

Abbreviations: C, college students; HS, high school students; IMse, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards stimulating experiences; IMa, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards achievement; IMk, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards knowledge; EMidr, Extrinsic Motivation identified regulation; EMintr, Extrinsic Motivation introjected regulation; EMer, Extrinsic Motivation external regulation; A, Amotivation.

Motivational differences between educational levels

Finally, differences with statistical significance were found in the high school and college student groups in all subscales, using t-tests. While high school students had higher averages in the EM and amotivation subscales, college students obtained higher scores in IM subscales (Table 7).

Table 7

Differences in motivational types according to educational level

IMseIMaIMkEMidrEMintrEMerA
High school
M8.9369.96110.33513.12411.51610.4175.00
 ST3.0032.9952.7922.2812.8751.8621.883
College
M10.96011.50013.13312.7897.7578.4004.306
 ST2.5572.7292.3882.3092.6482.3281.069
t−9.787−7.160−14.5281.96618.14813.0496.277
P-value<0.0001<0.0001<0.0001<0.0001<0.0001<0.0001<0.0001

Open in a separate window

Abbreviations: IMse, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards stimulating experiences; IMa, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards achievement; IMk, Intrinsic Motivation orientation towards knowledge; EMidr, Extrinsic Motivation identified regulation; EMintr, Extrinsic Motivation introjected regulation; EMer, Extrinsic Motivation external regulation; A, Amotivation.

Discussion

Supported by self-determination theory, the AMS probably constitutes the most used instrument for the assessment of student motivation.14 It has been used in many countries and versions in different languages are available. Despite this, in Latin America, cultural and linguistic differences often entail serious problems for comprehension and interpretation of some items. These difficulties affect the scale’s psychometric strengths directly. The experience of applying the scale in the context of high school and college education in Argentina shows the need for increased efforts in developing specific versions for this country.

Following the recommendations by Núñez et al on the convenience of reviewing AMS elements in a Latin American context, a pilot study and an examination by expert judges were performed,20 which resulted in a reformulation of some items and a review of the type of responses used. These tasks were executed with the intention of obtaining two versions of the AMS with similar contents, but adapted to the situational conditions for high school and college educational levels, taking into account local linguistic peculiarities (Appendices 1 and 2).

Psychometric analyses verified acceptable qualities for the instrument in both cases. Internal consistency values were very similar to previous versions where EMidr presented lower alphas.1620 As co*kley et al and Núñez et al proposed, this is possibly due to the fact that this EM subscale is most proximate to IM, something that could lead to some overlapping between them.17,20

From a perspective of the factorial structure of the scale, as found by Barkoukis et al, co*kley et al, Fairchild et al, and Núñez et al, a better fit for the seven-factor model was observed. A more detailed examination indicated an even better fit when leaving out an item (Item 27).1618,21

An element of discussion arises in the relations within the dimensions that compose the scale. The present investigation has found that, in general, the highest correlations occur between the closest dimensions in the self-determination continuum, as proposed theoretically.1520 In addition to this, it seems logical that, in contrast to EM subscales, IM orientations showed higher correlation coefficients since their origin is internal and, consequently, more hom*ogenous. More diversity is expected to be found in EM regulations since they come from external sources.

Nevertheless, some important theoretical deviations, as have been reported for the Spanish and Paraguayan versions, were found. EMidr correlated higher with EMer than with EMintr despite not being proximal in the theoretical continuum.19,20 A possible explanation is the one offered by co*kley et al, who pointed out that the division between EM and IM would be less sharp than the one originally postulated by Deci and Ryan.13,17

When examining profile differences between the high school and college student groups, it was found that college students showed a strong intrinsic prevalence (IM), while the younger students had higher averages in the EM and Amotivation subscales. This is coherent with the educational system characteristics, taking into account that in Argentina high school is mandatory and college studies are of personal choice. These results are consistent with those of other investigations.32 Ratelle et al also found that college students appeared to be predominantly regulated in an autonomous manner, but the motivational profiles of high school students combined different degrees of IM and EM.24

As for this study limitation, it is clear that the availability of a major sample would grant strength to the obtained results, especially when contemplating the generalization of the factorial structure found. The use of the ULS method and a bootstrap procedure attempted to counterbalance this sampling limitation and the lack of normality observed in the data distribution. Either way, results remain coherent with those in the literature.1621

Conclusions

The present investigation was planned with the intention of solving a clear problem of application and providing a measure for assessing motivation in the academic milieu that met required psychometric properties. In spite of the abundant existing literature about self-determination theory and the numerous studies centered in the AMS, researchers have not discussed the convenience of having versions of the scale properly adapted and validated from a linguistic and contextual point of view. In this sense, the Argentine case does not constitute an exception. This empirical work and these psychometric results make available two versions of the scale to be used with high school and college students to Argentine researchers. Furthermore, one of the values of this study is the validation of a version for high school students, a population with fewer precedents than the college student group.16,21

It is known that adapted scales provide psychometric quality, but restrict comparisons between populations since they are assessed by different measures. This aspect constitutes a limitation for comparative research and implies an advance in terms of operationalization of the construct in specific contexts. Far from stating parallelism or equivalence, an aspiration that exceeds the goals pursued here, achieving similar versions comparable in terms of content and applicable to high school and college student groups, is a concrete step forward for research and classroom evaluations. Adapted scales underlines its utility for descriptive studies in local populations, in developmental research, and studies of intervention efficacy. The possible use in individual or institutional monitoring is highlighted. This is important because motivation is associated with academic achievement and psychological adjustment.2224,32

However, future investigations should aim to improve these contributions by adding evidence of convergent validity and inquiring into the comparability of the versions.

Appendix

Appendix 1

Academic Motivation Scale: high school version (in Argentine Spanish)

¿Por qué vas al colegio?Totalmente en desacuerdoUn poco de acuerdoBastante de acuerdoTotalmente de acuerdo
1. Porque disfruto debatiendo/comunicando/escribiendo mis ideas a otros.
2. Por la satisfacción que experimento mientras me supero a mí misma/o en mis estudios.
3. Porque disfruto aprendiendo cosas nuevas.
4. Porque pienso que la educación secundaria me ayudará a estar mejor preparada/o para el proyecto de vida que decida.
5. Porque cuando tengo éxito en el colegio me siento importante.
6. Porque se necesita por lo menos un título secundario para encontrar un trabajo bien pago en el futuro.
7. Honestamente, no lo sé; realmente siento que estoy perdiendo el tiempo en el colegio.
8. Por el placer que experimento cuando participo en debates interesante con algunos profesores.
9. Por la satisfacción que experimento mientras me supero a mí misma/o en mis metas personales.
10. Porque me gusta descubrir nuevos temas que nunca antes había visto.
11. Porque es posible que me permita entrar en el mercado laboral en el campo que me gusta.
12. Porque me gusta tener buenas notas y que me feliciten por eso.
13. Para obtener un trabajo más prestigioso en el futuro.
14. Hace un tiempo tenía razones para ir al colegio; sin embargo, ahora me pregunto si continuar o no.
15. Por el placer de leer sobre temas que me interesan.
16. Por la satisfacción que siento cuando logro llevar a cabo actividades académicas difíciles.
17. Porque disfruto cuando aumento mi conocimiento sobre temas que me atraen.
18. Porque, en nuestra sociedad, es importante ir al colegio.
19. Porque no quiero ser un/a fracasado/a.
20. Para tener un mejor sueldo en el futuro.
21. No puedo entender por qué voy al colegio y, francamente, me importa muy poco.
22. Por la satisfacción de hacer algo que me gusta, como por ejemplo, escribir un cuento en Castellano, o hacer un experimento en Biología, o preparar un proyecto o monografía, etc.
23. Porque la escuela secundaria me permite experimentar un logro personal en mi búsqueda de la excelencia en mis estudios.
24. Porque mis estudios me permiten continuar aprendiendo muchas cosas que me interesan.
25. Porque creo que mi educación secundaria mejorará mis capacidades como trabajador/a.
26. Porque no quiero decepcionar a mi familia.
27. No lo sé; no puedo entender qué hago en el colegio.

Open in a separate window

Appendix 2

Academic Motivation Scale: college version (in Argentine Spanish)

¿Por qué vas a la facultad?Totalmente en desacuerdoUn poco de acuerdoBastante de acuerdoTotalmente de acuerdo
1. Porque disfruto debatiendo/comunicando/escribiendo mis ideas a otros.
2. Por la satisfacción que experimento mientras me supero a mí misma/o en mis estudios.
3. Porque disfruto aprendiendo cosas nuevas.
4. Porque creo que sin educación universitaria estaré poco preparada/o para trabajar en el área que me gusta, ya que no es lo mismo hacer una carrera universitaria que un curso o un terciario corto
5. Porque cuando tengo éxito en la facultad me siento importante
6. Porque se necesita algo más que un título secundario para encontrar un trabajo bien pago en el futuro
7. Honestamente, no lo sé; realmente siento que estoy perdiendo el tiempo en la facultad
8. Por el placer que experimento cuando participo en debates interesante con algunos profesores.
9. Por la satisfacción que experimento mientras me supero a mí misma/o en mis metas personales.
10. Porque me gusta descubrir nuevos temas, relacionados con mis intereses, que nunca antes había visto
11. Porque me permitirá entrar en el mercado laboral en el campo que me gusta
12. Porque me gusta tener buenas notas y que me feliciten por eso
13. Para obtener un trabajo más prestigioso en el futuro.
14. Hace un tiempo tenía razones para ir a la facultad; sin embargo, ahora me pregunto si continuar o no
15. Por el placer de leer sobre temas que me interesan.
16. Por la satisfacción que siento cuando logro llevar a cabo actividades académicas difíciles.
17. Porque disfruto cuando aumento mi conocimiento sobre temas que me atraen.
18. Porque, en nuestra sociedad, es importante ir a la facultad
19. Porque no quiero ser un/a fracasado/a.
20. Para tener un mejor sueldo en el futuro.
21. No puedo entender por qué voy a la facultad y, francamente, me importa muy poco
22. Por la satisfacción de hacer algo que me gusta relacionado con mi futura profesión (como por ejemplo, escribir un buen análisis de un tema/caso, hacer una maqueta, un experimento, etc)
23. Porque la facultad me permite experimentar un logro personal en la búsqueda de la excelencia en mis estudios
24. Porque mis estudios me permiten continuar aprendiendo muchas cosas que me interesan.
25. Porque creo que estos estudios mejorarán mis capacidades como trabajador/a
26. Porque no quiero decepcionar a mi familia.
27. No lo sé; no puedo entender qué hago en la facultad

Open in a separate window

Footnotes

Disclosures

The present investigation has been carried out with endorsem*nt of Buenos Aires University (UBA) and the National Council of Scientific and Technical Research (CONICET). The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

1. Côté JE, Levine CG. Attitude versus aptitude: Is intelligence or motivation more important for positive higher-educational outcomes? J Adolesc Res. 2000;15(1):58–80. [Google Scholar]

2. Miñano P, Gilar R, Castejón JL. Un modelo estructural de variables cognitivo motivacionales explicativas del rendimiento académico en lengua española y matemáticas. [An structural model of motivational cognitive variables that explain academic achievement in Spanish language and mathematics.] Anales de Psicología. 2012;28(1):45–54. [Google Scholar]

3. Murphy PK, Alexander PA. A motivated exploration of motivation terminology. Contemp Educ Psychol. 2000;25:3–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

4. Pintrich PR, De Groot EV. Motivational and self-regulated learning components of academic performance. J Educ Psychol. 1990;82(1):32–40. [Google Scholar]

5. Pintrich PR. The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In: Boekaerts M, Pintrich PR, Zeidner M, editors. Handbook of Self- Regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2000. pp. 452–502. [Google Scholar]

6. Gil P, Bernaras E, Elizalde LM, Arrieta M. Estrategias de aprendizaje y patrones de motivación del alumnado de cuatro titulaciones del campus de Gipuzkoa. [Learning strategies and motivational patterns of students of four career in the Gipuzkoa campus.] Infancia y Aprendizaje. 2009;32(3):329–341. [Google Scholar]

7. Torrano Montalvo F, González Torres MC. El aprendizaje autorregulado: Presente y futuro de la investigación. [Self-regulated learning: Present and future of research.] Rev Electrón Investig Psicoeduc. 2004;2(1):1–34. [Google Scholar]

8. Salim SR. Motivaciones, enfoques y estrategias de aprendizaje en estudiantes de Bioquímica de una universidad pública Argentina. [Motivations, approaches and strategies for learning in students of a university biochemistry at Argentina.] Rev Electróne Investig Educ. 2006;8(1):1–16. [Google Scholar]

9. Uriel F, Carreras MA, Ongarato P, Stover J, Fernández Liporace M. Estrategias de estudio y aprendizaje en estudiantes de nivel medio y universitario. [Study and learning strategies in high school and college students.] Acta Psiquiátr Psicol Am Lat. 2011;57(3):178–188. [Google Scholar]

10. Oliver MC, Eimer GA, Bálsamo NF, Crivello ME. Permanecia y abandono en química general en las carreras de ingeniería de la Universidad Tecnológica Nacional-Facultad Regional Córdoba (UTN-FRC), Argentina. [Permanence and neglect in general chemistry in the engineering of the National Technological University, Cordoba Regional Faculty.] Avances en Ciencia e Ingeniería. 2011;2(2):117–129. [Google Scholar]

11. Vázquez SM. Rendimiento académico y patrones de aprendizaje en estudiantes de ingeniería. [Academic performance and learning patterns in engineering students.] Ingeniería y Universidad. 2009;13(1):105–136. [Google Scholar]

12. Barca A, Porto AM, Santorum R, Barca E. Determinantes motivacionales y aprendizaje en el alumnado de educación secundaria con alto y bajo rendimiento académico. Un análisis desde la diversidad. [Motivational determinants and learning in high school students with high and low academic achievement: an analysis of the diversity.] In: Barca Lozano A, editor. Motivación y Aprendizaje en Contextos Educativos. Granada, Spain: Grupo Editorial Universitario; 2009. pp. 59–106. [Google Scholar]

13. Deci EL, Ryan RM. Intrinsic Motivation and Self-determination in Human Behavior. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]

14. Vallerand RJ, Blais MR, Briere NM, Pelletier LG. Construction et validation de l’Echelle de Motivation en Éducation (EME). [Construction and validation of the Ëchelle de Motivation en Éducation (EME).] Can J Behav Sci. 1998;21:323–349. [Google Scholar]

15. Vallerand RJ, Pelletier LG, Blais MR, Briere NM, Senécal C, Vallierès EF. The Academic Motivation Scale: a measure of intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivation in education. Educ Psychol Meas. 1992;52:1003–1017. [Google Scholar]

16. Barkoukis V, Tsorbatzoudis H, Grouios G, Sideridis G. The assessment of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and amotivation: Validity and reliability of the Greek version of the academic motivation scale. Assessment in Education: Principles, Policy and Practice. 2008;15(1):39–55. [Google Scholar]

17. co*kley KO, Bernard N, Cunningham D, Motoike J. A psychometric investigation of the academic motivation scale using a United States sample. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development. 2001;34:109–119. [Google Scholar]

18. Fairchild AJ, Horst SJ, Finney SJ, Barron KE. Evaluating existing and new validity evidence for the Academic Motivation Scale. Contemp Educ Psychol. 2005;30(3):331–358. [Google Scholar]

19. Núñez JL, Martín-Albo J, Navarro JG. Validación de la versión española de la Ëchelle de Motivation en Éducation. Psicothema. 2005;17(2):334–349. [Google Scholar]

20. Núñez JL, Martín-Albo J, Navarro JG, Grijalvo F. Validación de la Escala de Motivación Educativa (EME) en Paraguay. [Validation of the Spanish Version of the Ëchelle de Motivation en Éducation.] Interam J Psychol. 2006;40(2):185–192. [Google Scholar]

21. Nuñez JL, Martín-Albo J, Navarro JG, Suárez Z. Adaptación y validación de la versión española de la Escala de Motivación Educativa en estudiantes de educación secundaria postobligatoria. [Adaptation and validation of the Spanish version of the Academic Motivation Scale in high school students.] Estudios de Psicología. 2010;31(1):89–100. [Google Scholar]

22. Ahmed W, Bruinsma MA. Structural model of self-concept, autonomous motivation and academic performance in cross-cultural perspective. Rev Electron Investig Psicoeduc Psigopedag. 2006;4(3):551–576. [Google Scholar]

23. Legault L, Green Demers I, Pelletier L. Why do high school students lack motivation in the classroom? Toward an understanding of amotivation and the role of social support. J Educ Psychol. 2006;98(3):567–582. [Google Scholar]

24. Ratelle CF, Guay F, Vallerand RJ, Larose S, Senécal C. Autonomous, controlled, and amotivated types of academic motivation: A person-oriented analysis. J Educ Psychol. 2007;99(4):734–746. [Google Scholar]

25. Vallerand RJ, Fortier MS, Guay F. Self-determination and persistence in a real-life setting: toward a motivational model of high school dropout. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997;72(5):1161–1176. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

26. Ministerio de Educación de la Nación. Anuario Estadístico Educativo. Buenos Aires, Argentina: Ministerio de Educación; 2010. [National Ministry of Education. Annual Educational Statistics.] [Google Scholar]

27. Elosúa P, Zumbo BD. Coeficientes de confiabilidad para escalas de respuesta categórica ordenada. [Fiability coefficients of ordinal data.] Psicothema. 2008;20(4):896–901. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

28. Forero CG, Maydeu-Olivares A, Gallardo-Pujol D. Factor analysis with ordinal indicators: A Monte Carlo study comparing DWLS and ULS estimation. Struct Equ Modeling. 2009;16(4):625–641. [Google Scholar]

29. Ximénez C. Effect of variable and subject sampling on recovery of weak factors in CFA. Methodology. 2007;3(2):67–80. [Google Scholar]

30. Byrne BM. Structural Equation Modeling with AMOS. New York, NY: Routledge; 2009. [Google Scholar]

31. Kline RB. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York, NY: Guilford; 2005. [Google Scholar]

32. Ratelle CF, Guay F, Larose S, Senécal C. Family correlates of trajectories of academic motivation during a school transition: A semiparametric group-based approach. J Educ Psychol. 2004;96(4):743–754. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Psychology Research and Behavior Management are provided here courtesy of Dove Press

Academic Motivation Scale: adaptation and psychometric analyses for high school and college students (2024)
Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Jamar Nader

Last Updated:

Views: 6372

Rating: 4.4 / 5 (75 voted)

Reviews: 82% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Jamar Nader

Birthday: 1995-02-28

Address: Apt. 536 6162 Reichel Greens, Port Zackaryside, CT 22682-9804

Phone: +9958384818317

Job: IT Representative

Hobby: Scrapbooking, Hiking, Hunting, Kite flying, Blacksmithing, Video gaming, Foraging

Introduction: My name is Jamar Nader, I am a fine, shiny, colorful, bright, nice, perfect, curious person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.